As the early stages of learning commence, from toddlerhood to adolescence, imitation has been the primary learning technique for the development of speech, understanding reactions to external stimuli, and emotional responses to situations. As the development and evolution of human beings began, these behaviours of observational learning and imitation can be seen to be deeply rooted in human psychology. If we shift our focus to the question of why children copy adults, we will gain insight into how they acquire language, learn social norms, develop empathy, and build their sense of identity. The psychology of imitation thus becomes an important aspect of self-formation and self-concept.
Imitation as a Core Learning Mechanism
Psychologist Albert Bandura’s groundbreaking research of 1961, the Bobo doll experiment, is also known for being a part of Bandura’s social learning theory. This research study demonstrated that children learn not only through direct experience but also by observing and mimicking the behaviours and responses of others. It also assessed that there are no negative consequences. Children who watched an adult act aggressively toward a doll were more likely to replicate that aggression themselves. This led to the formation of Social Learning Theory, which proposes that behaviour is learned through observation, imitation, and modelling.
Children, through observational learning, imitate both positive and negative behaviours. Adults, especially caregivers, parents and teachers, perform as powerful role models and they often, unintentionally shape children’s actions, behaviour and attitudes. As Bandura emphasised, reinforcement isn’t always necessary for learning; simply seeing a behaviour can be enough for a child to internalise it.
Imitation Starts in Infancy
Research by Meltzoff and Moore (1977) exhibits that imitation begins in very early stages of development. Through this study, researchers have found that newborns as young as two weeks old could mimic facial expressions like tongue protrusion and mouth opening. This Early imitation suggests that humans are biologically equipped to learn through social interaction from birth.
Andrew Meltzoff later proposed the “Like Me” hypothesis, arguing that imitation helps infants connect with others by mapping observed behaviours onto their bodies. This process is not just about copying but about understanding the intentions and emotions behind the action, a foundational element of social cognition.
The Role of Mirror Neurons
Neurosciences purvey further explanation through the discovery of mirror neurons, which fire both when a person acts and when they observe someone else performing it. This mirroring mechanism helps explain why imitation feels so natural because the brain is wired to connect perception and action. For children, this neural setup enables them to quickly absorb and repeat the behaviours they observe in adults.
Cultural Learning and Shared Intentions
Children’s imitation is not random; it is often selective and purposeful. According to Tomasello et al. (2005), young children do not just copy surface behaviour; they are trying to understand the goals and intentions behind those actions. This capacity to engage in shared intentionality is something that allows human beings to pass down complex cultural knowledge across generations.
Similarly, Gergely and Csibra (2005) posited the theory of natural pedagogy, which suggests that children are peculiarly sensitive to cues that an adult is trying to teach them something. The use of gestures, eye contact, or a certain tone of voice works as an alarm for children to recognise the opportunity to learn, making them more likely to imitate the behaviour.
Imitation as Identity Formation
Imitation also plays a consequential role in the development of self-concept. Children use mimicry for understanding and differentiating roles and behaviours as they form their own identities. It is seen that children pretend to be a superhero, a teacher or imitate their parents, which allows them to explore social roles and emotional responses safely and playfully.
Read More: Exploring Human Connection: A Look at Attachment Theory
Conclusion
Imitation is a core of human development, which stands as an instinctive, powerful tool that children use to learn, connect, and grow. From Bandura’s theories to Meltzoff’s infant studies and Tomasello’s insights on cultural learning, the psychology of imitation reveals just how vital adult behaviour is in shaping young minds. By being conscious of their actions, adults can foster environments that encourage empathy, curiosity, and positive growth simply by being the kind of person they hope their children will become.
FAQs
1. What is imitation psychology?
In psychology, imitation refers to the act of copying the behaviour, actions, or gestures of another person or model, whether consciously or unconsciously. It’s a fundamental aspect of learning, especially in social contexts, and plays a role in developing skills, habits, and even cultural traditions.
2. What is imitation learning in psychology?
Learning by imitation, also known as observational learning, is a key process in psychology where individuals acquire new behaviours, skills, or knowledge by observing and replicating the actions of others. This type of learning is crucial for human development, enabling us to learn from those around us, including parents, caregivers, siblings, and peers.
3. What is the concept of imitation?
Imitation is the act of copying or mimicking the behaviour, appearance, or style of someone else or something else. It can involve replicating actions, sounds, words, or even the overall aesthetic of a model.
References +
Watson, A. L. (2009). Monkey see, monkey do: Imitation as a mechanism in children’s social learning (Master’s thesis, Georgia State University). ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/psych_theses/119/
• Meltzoff, A. N. (2013). Meltzoff’s ‘Like Me’ theory. In M. R. Banaji & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 139–144). Oxford University Press. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/304183953_Meltzoff’s_Like_Me_Theory
• Quizlet. (n.d.). Module 30 study guide questions [Flashcard set]. Quizlet. https://quizlet.com/557059243/module-30-study-guide-questions-flash-cards
• Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675–691. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000129
• Heyes, C. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(6), 253–261. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01586-0
Leave feedback about this