Can you Survive the ‘Cue’ test?
Positive Research

Can you Survive the ‘Cue’ test?

survival-of-the-cue-test

Have you ever wondered why we’re drawn to the sweet voices of children or the innocent things they say in their playful ways? Is it simply their cuteness that captivates us, or could there be a deeper, perhaps evolutionary, explanation behind it? Research has shown that certain cues, whether facial or vocal, expressed by children, elicit certain kinds of responses from the caregiver. These traits—such as their innocent voices, facial expressions, and imaginative thinking—enhance their chances of receiving the care and attention they need for survival. Over time, evolution has equipped children with characteristics that trigger emotional responses in adults, fostering protective behaviours and encouraging caregiving.

A study by Hernández Blasi, Bjorklund, Agut, Lozano Nomdedeu, and Martínez delves into how children’s voices and cognitive styles—whether magical or logical—shape adult perceptions, revealing that certain cues are more effective in eliciting caregiving responses. By investigating how adults assess children’s emotional vulnerability, intelligence, and overall need for care, this research provides valuable insights into the underlying mechanisms of human caregiving. It highlights how evolutionary adaptations have fine-tuned these interactions, ensuring that children receive the attention crucial for their growth and development.

Why Children’s Traits Matter

Evolutionarily speaking, the survival of children has been a central focus in the existence of the human race. We don’t give birth to a litter or reproduce as quickly as other species; instead, we invest heavily in a few offspring. In such a situation, it becomes imperative to protect the few that are born and to choose the healthier ones to receive that protection. Historically, infant mortality rates were very high, with nearly half of all children not surviving puberty. This reality forced parents, particularly mothers, to be selective about how much time, effort, and resources they devoted to each child. Overinvesting in a sickly child could mean neglecting the needs of healthier siblings or future offspring.

Natural selection has led to the development of adaptations in parents that help them assess the fitness of their offspring. These adaptations aid in making decisions about when and how much to invest in a child. For example, historical evidence indicates that sickly infants might have faced abandonment or infanticide, highlighting the difficult choices parents had to make when resources were scarce.

In this process of receiving parental care and affection, children aren’t passive recipients; instead, they have developed “psychological weapons” to enhance their appeal to parents. Robert Trivers introduced the idea of “psychological weapons,” which are traits and behaviours that children have developed to influence how much care and resources they receive from their parents. He argues that children actively engage with their parents to get more than what parents might want to give. This creates a conflict because children seek as much investment as possible to enhance their
chances of survival, while parents have to consider the needs of all their offspring.

For example, babies might cry in a specific way that grabs their parents’ attention, encouraging them to respond quickly for care and resources. They may also time their requests strategically to maximize parental responses, such as asking for food when parents are likely to give in. Since children are genetically identical to themselves but only partially related to their siblings, they may prioritize their own needs over those of their brothers and sisters, which adds to the conflict.

Trivers relates this behaviour to the concept of inclusive fitness, introduced by W.D. Hamilton, which helps explain how offspring weigh the benefits of receiving more care against the potential impact on their siblings. This means that children have evolved to use manipulation to increase their chances of survival, highlighting the complicated relationships between parents and children, where both sides have different interests that sometimes clash.

Comparing Vocal and Cognitive Cues

The study by Hernández Blasi, Bjorklund, Agut, Lozano Nomdedeu, and Martínez aims to determine which of these two cues elicits a more favourable response from the caregiver and the nature of that response. There are primarily three types of cues – baby faces, vocal cues and magical thinking.

Baby Faces

Konrad Lorenz, a renowned ethologist, introduced the concept of a “baby schema” (‘Kindchenschema’), which suggests that specific infantile physical characteristics, such as a large head, round face, and big eyes, are universally perceived as cute. This perception triggers caretaking behaviour in others, contributing to the survival and well-being of offspring. These features are believed to have evolved to elicit caregiving responses, ensuring the protection and nurturing of infants in various species.

A more relevant study by Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald in 1979 used actual photographs of infants to measure how specific facial features correlate with perceived cuteness. Their results indicated that cuter infants tend to have large eyes, a large forehead, and shorter, narrower facial features. Researchers have supported this idea through studies on human preferences for baby faces. For example, both children and adults tend to prefer looking at pictures of infants over those of adults.

This preference has been capitalized on in industries like film and advertising, where characters are designed with exaggerated baby-like features to appear more appealing (e.g., Mickey Mouse). Most of the baby schema’s features are found in an infant’s head and face, and many earlier studies have used simplified representations—such as line drawings or schematic faces—to test this concept. These simplified stimuli, like outlines of facial shapes, have confirmed that certain features, like a large head and large eyes, trigger perceptions of cuteness and caregiving responses.

As infants grow older, the perceived cuteness of their faces begins to decline. This suggests that as their facial features mature and become less “baby-like,” caregivers’ perception of the child as helpless and in need of care diminishes. In other words, the cute, immature facial features that originally trigger caregiving responses fade as the child ages, and caregivers begin to view them as more independent and less in need of constant protection or attention. The visual cues that once signalled a need for care and support become less pronounced as the child develops.

Vocal Cues

Belin, Fecteau, and Bedard (2004) explained how the human voice is an “auditory face”, meaning just like a face can convey important non-verbal cues through expressions, voices can provide key information about a person’s emotions, identity, and needs. Through separate but interconnected
pathways, our brain processes vocal information. The pathways used in vocal recognition are similar to that of facial recognition. Our brains possess specialized systems for analyzing vocal cues, allowing us to efficiently interpret complex social signals, just as we have specific neural areas for interpreting facial expressions and identities aiding our social interaction and communication.

In the context of infants, crying is an important vocal cue that conveys information crucial for its survival. Crying for infants, is a way, for them to be able to express their needs and emotional states. At around three months of age, the acoustic characteristics of a baby’s cries can effectively guide adult behaviour, prompting caregivers to respond appropriately to the infant’s distress or needs (Wolff, 1969). Parents are particularly attuned to variations in their infants’ cries, which can communicate different levels of urgency or specific needs, such as hunger or discomfort (Chittora & Patil, 2017). This increased sensitivity highlights the importance of vocal communication in caregiving.

Read More: Mastering Effective Communication: Building Trust and Strong Relationships

It emphasizes that both voice and face processing are crucial for effective social interaction and bonding between infants and their caregivers. These mechanisms ensure that caregivers can promptly respond to their infants, increasing the chances of survival and fostering strong emotional connections.

Magical Thinking

Magical thinking is a natural process of children’s cognitive development. It is a process through which they find a causal link or explanation between events that are generally not interlinked. This type of thinking often involves believing that one’s thoughts, wishes, or actions can influence the physical world in ways that defy the laws of cause and effect.

This type of reasoning in young children evokes positive feelings in adults. Research has shown that supernatural thinking (e.g., animism and finalism), are perceived more positively than natural thinking (e.g., overestimation and poor inhibition) (Bjorklund et al., 2010; Periss et al., 2012). In
supernatural thinking a child attributes human-like motives and intentions to non-human entities, reflecting their imaginative way of understanding the world around them. For example, a child may say, “The wind is blowing because it’s trying to help the leaves dance.”

Read More: Role of Play in Child Development and Emotional Expression

Other forms of magical thinking include beliefs like animism—where children attribute life to inanimate objects, such as thinking the sun is “mad” when it isn’t shining—which helps them express their emotions and make sense of natural phenomena. Similarly, finalism—believing that everything occurs for a specific purpose—encourages children to seek meaning in their experiences, promoting curiosity and imaginative thinking. Additionally, childhood beliefs, such as those in Santa Claus or the idea of becoming invisible by hiding, foster creativity and enhance social interactions, allowing children to bond with peers through shared fantasies.

In contrast, natural thinking is based on reasoning without supernatural elements, where children may overestimate their abilities or struggle to control their responses (Diamond, 2013; Harnishfeger, 1995). Adults tend to feel more positive and less negative toward children who display magical thinking, regardless of the children’s age (Bjorklund et al., 2010). This type of thinking not only provides insight into a child’s intelligence and vulnerability but also affects adults’ empathetic responses toward them (Hernández Blasi et al., 2022; Hernández Blasi et al., 2015, 2017).

In contrast, natural thinking is based on reasoning without supernatural elements, where children may overestimate their abilities or struggle to control their responses (Diamond, 2013; Harnishfeger, 1995). Adults tend to feel more positive and less negative toward children who display magical thinking, regardless of the children’s age (Bjorklund et al., 2010). This type of thinking not only provides insight into a child’s intelligence and vulnerability but also affects adults’ empathetic responses toward them (Hernández Blasi et al., 2022; Hernández Blasi et al., 2015, 2017).

The positive bias toward this kind of thinking could be due to its humorous nature, the potential to promote good behaviour, and its evolutionary link to cooperation (Bjorklund et al., 2010). Furthermore, magical thinking helps adults and older adolescents make sense of children’s behaviour (Yong et al., 2021), which may lead to increased empathy toward children who engage in it, especially in older adolescents who are approaching parenthood (Hernández Blasi & Bjorklund, 2018; Periss et al., 2012).

Research Findings

In the research done by Hernández Blasi, Bjorklund, Agut, Lozano Nomdedeu, and Martínez it was found that adults tend to exhibit greater positive emotions and perceptions of helplessness toward children whose vocal and cognitive cues are congruent. For example, children demonstrating immature thinking, regardless of whether it’s supernatural or natural, elicit higher positive affect and perceptions of helplessness. However, these children are also deemed less intelligent, indicating a complex relationship between perceived maturity and emotional responses.

On the other hand, when children’s vocal expressions do not match their cognitive cues, as in the case of inconsistent conditions, adults may prioritize vocal signals, particularly regarding positive affect and helplessness. This highlights the importance of vocal cues in shaping adult perceptions of children’s needs and emotions, emphasizing that the context in which these cues are presented significantly influences adult judgments.

Moreover, the study uncovers that in inconsistent conditions, where children’s cognitive and vocal cues diverge, adult evaluations can shift dramatically. For instance, while children’s vocal characteristics generally dominate perceptions of positive affect and helplessness, cognitive attributes take precedence when assessing intelligence, particularly in cases of supernatural thinking. This suggests that adults may rely on different signals depending on the situation, emphasizing the nuanced ways in which vocal and cognitive cues interact to inform perceptions of children’s emotional states and intelligence. Overall, the findings underscore the complexity of adult-child interactions, revealing that both vocal and cognitive characteristics are essential for understanding children’s needs, but their influence can vary depending on whether the cues are consistent or conflicting.

References +
  • Belin, P., Fecteau, S., & Bédard, C. (2004). Thinking the voice: Neural correlates of voice perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(3), 129-135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.008
  • Hildebrandt, K. A., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1979). Facial feature determinants of perceived infant attractiveness. Infant Behavior and Development, 2, 329-339. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(79)80043-0
  • Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., & Sachser, N. (2009). Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology, 115(3), 257–263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439- 0310.2008.01603.x
  • Hernández Blasi, C., Bjorklund, D. F., Agut, S., Lozano Nomdedeu, F., & Martínez, M. Á. (2024). Children’s evolved cues to promote caregiving: Are voices more powerful than thoughts in signaling young children’s attributes and needs to adults? Evolution and Human Behavior, 45(5), 106609. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2024.106609

Leave feedback about this

  • Rating
X